/ News

14.11.2013

How gullible are we when it comes to robots?

Humans readily anthropomorphise inanimate objects but research suggests even babies are surprisingly sensitive to the differences between robots and humans when it comes to social interaction.

The Tokyo international robot exhibition (iRex), which opened last Thursday, showcases the world’s latest robot technology and applications. Already robots are replacing humans in industries such as manufacturing, military battle and search and rescue. But there are increasing demands for robots that will interact with us in more intimate and social ways.

In August Kirobo, the talking robot, was sent to the international space station to explore how robots can be used to emotionally support humans in social isolation. Chris Melhuish, director of the Bristol Robotics Laboratory, considers one of the major drives of robotic development to be care and companionship for the elderly. And robotic toys for children having been growing steadily in popularity.

The success of these new technologies will rest in large part on our tendency to anthropomorphise objects - to think of them as having thoughts, intentions and emotions like humans. But just how gullible are we when it comes to treating robots as social partners?

Certainly we seem to readily anthropomorphise inanimate objects – most adults will have some experience of pleading with their computer not to crash or kicking their apparently uncooperative car. And anthropomorphism is driven more by how objects behave than how they look. Researchers at the University of Calgery showed that adults presented with a motorized balsa-wood stick with no personifying features none-the-less reported feeling scared, seduced or intimidated by it.

We see faces in cloudsproject personalities onto machines and assume intentions in even simple geometric figures. Animation and puppetry rely largely on this hair-trigger bias to treat moving objects as social entities. But most adults do not really believe that these objects are living, thinking beings in the same way that humans and animals are. How do we become sensitive to these sometimes subtle differences?

The early roots of anthropomorphism are evident even in very young infants. One of our first categorical distinctions is between living and non-living entities but eye-tracking and behavioural studies show that infants from around 6-months of age attribute thoughts and feelings to simple geometric figures, follow the gaze of robots to novel objects and treat simple automated boxes as having goals and intentions.

However, even very young infants have limited credulity when it comes to robots. In two papers published earlier this year, Yuko Okumura and colleagues at Kyoto University Baby Lab compared how 12-month-olds treat human and robot gaze direction. The robot they used had human-like eyes and hands but was clearly a robot.

Understanding eye-gaze as a learning cue is an important social development in infants that underlies their ability to learn labels for objects, how to use objects and how to predict other people’s goals. Previous research has shown that infants will follow the gaze of robots (but not a directional light or pointer) to an object and have taken this as evidence that they are treating robots as social entities.

However, Okumura’s studies showed there are significant differences. Only in the human condition do babies subsequently remember the object and imitate actions on it. The authors suggest that although babies are responding to robots in an anthropomorphic manner, at 12-months of age they are already sensitive to differences between human and robot communication as cues to learning.

Older children show similar sensitivity. Kahn and colleagues observed 3- and 5-year-olds interacting with a Sony AIBO robodog and a stuffed toy dog. A number of studies have shown that although young children readily attribute thoughts and feelings to their stuffed toys in imaginary play, they do not really believe that they are living social entities. The robodog, in contrast, had numerous features that might confuse children into thinking it was a living animal such as self-initiated movement, learning new behaviours and a repertoire of skills including sitting, standing and playing dead.

When interviewed the majority of children attributed the robodog with thoughts, feelings and intentions. However, children attributed the samemental states to the stuffed toy dog. Its possible then that the anthropomorphism exhibited towards the robodog is a similar form of make-believe to that engaged in with stuffed toys rather than children really confusing the robot with a social companion.

This was supported by a subsequent study showing that children treat a real dog and the robodog significantly differently – the real dog was attributed with more feelings, thoughts and moral rights and children spent more time touching it or in close proximity to it.

Khan and his co-authors propose that rather than thinking of robots as being ‘alive’ or ‘not-alive’, perhaps children and adults are creating a new mental category that lies somewhere in between. This might explain our spontaneous tendency to project personalities and feelings onto robots while simultaneously being happy to lock them in a cupboard for hours on end or send them into mine fields.

The fact that robots are treated as different from humans might prove to be a useful asset. For instance, numerous studies have shown that children with autism are more motivated to learnreadier to acquire basic social skills and are less intimidated in diagnostic situations when faced with a robot counterpart than a human one. Dr. Karen Guldberg at Birmingham University’s Autism Centre suggests that the key to robots’ success in these contexts is specifically because children with autism see them as something other than living things and therefore are not threatened by them.

As the technology advances and robots become more commonplace in our work-spaces and homes it is possible that we will become less sensitive to these differences. What current research is showing however is that even babies are more sophisticated in distinguishing between robots and humans than we previously thought. Understanding the psychological roots of our skepticism towards social robots is vitally important in developing robots that can eventually cater to our social requirements.

Source: http://www.theguardian.com/science/2013/nov/12/how-gullible-are-we-when-it-comes-to-robots




/ About us

Founded by Russian entrepreneur Dmitry Itskov in February 2011 with the participation of leading Russian specialists in the field of neural interfaces, robotics, artificial organs and systems.

The main goals of the 2045 Initiative: the creation and realization of a new strategy for the development of humanity which meets global civilization challenges; the creation of optimale conditions promoting the spiritual enlightenment of humanity; and the realization of a new futuristic reality based on 5 principles: high spirituality, high culture, high ethics, high science and high technologies. 

The main science mega-project of the 2045 Initiative aims to create technologies enabling the transfer of a individual’s personality to a more advanced non-biological carrier, and extending life, including to the point of immortality. We devote particular attention to enabling the fullest possible dialogue between the world’s major spiritual traditions, science and society.

A large-scale transformation of humanity, comparable to some of the major spiritual and sci-tech revolutions in history, will require a new strategy. We believe this to be necessary to overcome existing crises, which threaten our planetary habitat and the continued existence of humanity as a species. With the 2045 Initiative, we hope to realize a new strategy for humanity's development, and in so doing, create a more productive, fulfilling, and satisfying future.

The "2045" team is working towards creating an international research center where leading scientists will be engaged in research and development in the fields of anthropomorphic robotics, living systems modeling and brain and consciousness modeling with the goal of transferring one’s individual consciousness to an artificial carrier and achieving cybernetic immortality.

An annual congress "The Global Future 2045" is organized by the Initiative to give platform for discussing mankind's evolutionary strategy based on technologies of cybernetic immortality as well as the possible impact of such technologies on global society, politics and economies of the future.

 

Future prospects of "2045" Initiative for society

2015-2020

The emergence and widespread use of affordable android "avatars" controlled by a "brain-computer" interface. Coupled with related technologies “avatars’ will give people a number of new features: ability to work in dangerous environments, perform rescue operations, travel in extreme situations etc.
Avatar components will be used in medicine for the rehabilitation of fully or partially disabled patients giving them prosthetic limbs or recover lost senses.

2020-2025

Creation of an autonomous life-support system for the human brain linked to a robot, ‘avatar’, will save people whose body is completely worn out or irreversibly damaged. Any patient with an intact brain will be able to return to a fully functioning  bodily life. Such technologies will  greatly enlarge  the possibility of hybrid bio-electronic devices, thus creating a new IT revolution and will make  all  kinds of superimpositions of electronic and biological systems possible.

2030-2035

Creation of a computer model of the brain and human consciousness  with the subsequent development of means to transfer individual consciousness  onto an artificial carrier. This development will profoundly change the world, it will not only give everyone the possibility of  cybernetic immortality but will also create a friendly artificial intelligence,  expand human capabilities  and provide opportunities for ordinary people to restore or modify their own brain multiple times.  The final result  at this stage can be a real revolution in the understanding of human nature that will completely change the human and technical prospects for humanity.

2045

This is the time when substance-independent minds will receive new bodies with capacities far exceeding those of ordinary humans. A new era for humanity will arrive!  Changes will occur in all spheres of human activity – energy generation, transportation, politics, medicine, psychology, sciences, and so on.

Today it is hard to imagine a future when bodies consisting of nanorobots  will become affordable  and capable of taking any form. It is also hard to imagine body holograms featuring controlled matter. One thing is clear however:  humanity, for the first time in its history, will make a fully managed evolutionary transition and eventually become a new species. Moreover,  prerequisites for a large-scale  expansion into outer space will be created as well.

 

Key elements of the project in the future

• International social movement
• social network immortal.me
• charitable foundation "Global Future 2045" (Foundation 2045)
• scientific research centre "Immortality"
• business incubator
• University of "Immortality"
• annual award for contribution to the realization of  the project of "Immortality”.

Login as user:

If you are registered on one of these websites, you can get a quick registration. To do this, please select the wesite and follow the instructions.

Login to 2045.com

Email:
You do not have login to 2045.com? Register!
Dear colleagues, partners, friends! If you support ​the 2045 strategic social initiative goals and values, please register on our website.

Quick registration:

If you are registered on one of these websites, you can get a quick registration. To do this, please select the wesite and follow the instructions.

Registration

Name:
Surname:
Field of activity:
Email:
Password:
Enter the code shown:

Show another picture

Восстановить пароль

Email:

Text:
Contact Email:
Attachment ( not greater than 5 Mb. ):
 
Close
avatar project milestones